CAIRN-INT.INFO : International Edition

1Few quantitative surveys provide the necessary data to reconstitute the major episodes in individuals’ lives and then compare them against subjective judgements. The Biographies et Entourage survey was conducted by INED in 2001 on a sample of persons aged 50 to 70 living in the Paris region. It recorded the composition of their contact circle over their entire lifetimes, along with their retrospective assessments of the various periods they had lived through. In this article, Valérie Golaz and Éva Lelièvre set facts against perceptions and analyse judgements of the past. More specifically, they explore the influence of family situations on respondents’ perceptions of their childhood and adolescence. For men and women alike, the family circle has a significant influence on their view of the past. Situations where the family circle is very small (just one person in some cases), the death of a parent and experience of war all lead to more negative opinions of certain years or periods of childhood, and of adolescence in particular.

2Since the 1980s, the issue of intergenerational family solidarities has aroused unprecedented interest, both for researchers and in the public policy framework. However, in the context of the ageing of western societies, attention has tended to focus on forms of solidarity directed at older people, as demonstrated by the review of surveys on family support in Europe (Bonvalet and Ogg, 2006) and various research projects based on the SHARE survey data (Attias-Donfut and Sirven, 2009). The question of solidarity towards children and adolescents has stimulated other types of specialized research, notably on social services care for children at risk (Frechon et al., 2009; Frechon and Villeneuve-Gokalp, 2009) and approaches to preschool childcare (Pailhé and Solaz, 2009). However, another body of research has focused on describing the overall picture of family configurations, by considering the whole network beyond the household and including the close contact circle (Widmer and Jallinoja, 2008; Bonvalet and Lelièvre, 1995). These analyses reveal the existence of complex, unexpected intergenerational configurations, which involve very diverse parental relationships and figures. The educational and parental universes of children and adolescents provide evidence of intergenerational solidarities and in some cases reflect complementary or surrogate parenting roles, thus suggesting multi-parenting configurations (Tichit and Vivier, 2005).

3Continuing the line of research based on quantitative data from the Biographies et Entourage survey (Event histories and contact circle survey; Lelièvre and Vivier, 2001; Bonvalet and Lelièvre, 2012), we identify and describe the respondents’ close family circles during childhood and adolescence. Taking the approach of comparing facts to perceptions, this article aims to determine the degree to which the respondents’ assessments of their childhood and adolescent years (subjective information) is contingent on their type of contact circle during those periods. Our results relate to birth cohorts who experienced childhood and adolescence between 1930 and 1965 – a period which predates the changes affecting the family since the 1970s, but whose historical context includes the Second World War and the major economic transformations caused by France’s industrialization and rapid urbanization.

4Constructing and structuring life courses through the perceptions of those concerned is a promising approach to the study of life-event histories rarely explored by demographers; data from the Biographies et Entourage survey allow us to apply this approach in a unique way. Although some pioneering sociologists (Hareven, 1986; Leclerc-Olive, 1997) have captured the subjective quality of life-course narratives and developed the qualitative analysis of events, turning-points and bifurcations (Bessin et al., 2010), others whose work comes closer to clinical psychology have broadly engaged in reconstructing family configurations as cognitive contexts, sometimes even with a therapeutic aim (Widmer, 1999; Widmer et al., 2005). This type of research, starting from detailed in-depth qualitative interviews (Locke and Lloyd-Sherlock, 2011), sometimes targeting particular populations – people in psychotherapy (Widmer and Dumas, 2008) or those who have recently experienced marriage break-up (Solsona and Simó, 2009), for example – has integrated examination of the facts with the respondents’ perceptions or assessments of the situation. In our case, comparison between the respondents’ family situations and their subjective assessments of these periods is based on a broad quantitative sample and on a reconstruction of the composition of respondents’ family circles over their lifetimes. Naturally, histories collected in this way do not have the precision of those recorded in qualitative interviews; nevertheless, the representative, quantitative nature of the sample makes it possible to identify strong trends in the general population and to reveal regularities, by addressing questions on the family and contextual factors that influence the perceived quality of childhood and adolescence: Is having a small family circle a factor that leads to negative assessment? What gender differences can be observed?

5For the analysis of perceptions, moving beyond a simple assessment of childhood and adolescence (summarized as “a difficult period”, “good years” “problem-free years”, etc.), we will explore the respondents’ comments using textual analysis. We will use logistic regressions to analyse the statistical relationships between these assessments and the respondents’ family circle at the time, followed by multilevel analyses for a more detailed exploration.

I – The Biographies et Entourage survey: factual data and perceptions

6The Biographies et Entourage survey, carried out in 2000-2001 on residents of Île-de-France (Paris region) born between 1930 and 1950, reconstructed not only each respondent’s life course but also that of his/her contact circle. The contact circle, as recorded in the survey, consists of “imposed” figures from the respondent’s close family: biological parents, adoptive parents, father’s/mother’s spouse, other people who had played a parental role, [1] parents-in-law, spouses, siblings, step-siblings, children (both of the respondent and of his/her spouse) and grandchildren. The survey looks systematically at these figures, but also adds any other people who were co-resident with the respondent for at least one year, as well as a certain number of key figures who were mentioned for the important – positive or negative – role they had played in the respondent’s life (Table 1).

Table 1

Information about contact circle from the Biographies et Entourage survey

Table 1
Type of relationship explored Universe Information used Family figures systematically covered by the survey Parents and parent figures Important dates for the relationship between this person and ego Frequency of contact at time of survey Spatial proximity Siblings / step-siblings Spouses / parents-in-law Ego’s children / his/her spouse’s children Grandchildren Co-residents Any person who lived with ego for at least one year Years of co-residence Other important people who played a positive or negative role in ego’s life Mentioned by ego Periods of influence: Childhood Adolescence Later

Information about contact circle from the Biographies et Entourage survey

Source: Biographies et Entourage survey, 2001.

Box 1. Wording of summary questions at the end of the Biographies et Entourage survey questionnaire

Sy 1 - Can you divide your life into different periods?
Describe these periods, identifying what differentiates them from each other and what they represent in your life.
Sy 2 - For each period, were these:
VG very good years
G good years
PF problem-free years
D difficult years
VD very difficult years

7At the end of the event history interviews and once all the factual milestones in the respondents’ life course and those of the members of their contact circle had been reconstructed, respondents were asked to divide their life into different periods and to place each one on a five-point assessment scale from VD (very difficult) to VG (very good), then to explain this breakdown by describing each of the periods defined (Box 1). The survey data therefore gave us the respondents’ assessments of the different periods in their lives, including those longest ago – childhood and adolescence. Delimiting these periods is in itself a thorny question (Attias-Donfut, 1991; Galambos et al., 2005). For demographers dealing with quantitative data, the commonly used criteria are based on reproductive biology – with the period of childhood extending from birth to puberty (Henry and van de Walle, 1982) – on the subjects’ economic and emotional “independence” as gauged by schooling and income, and on legal age limits (Meslé et al., 2011), making it possible to fix a common age for the entire sample population. In this way, the period of childhood was defined here as finishing at the end of the twelfth year – the age at which some young people in these birth cohorts had already taken up an apprenticeship. This also corresponds to the mean age of menarche for the girls and to the early onset of puberty for the boys. Their adolescence began at age 13 and extended until the legal age of majority for people born between 1930 and 1950, i.e. 21 years. All 2,830 contact circle trajectories available from the Biographies et Entourage survey were used in conducting this research.

8These data enabled us, firstly, to reconstruct the composition of these people’s contact circles over their lifetimes and, secondly, to provide a retrospective assessment of the different years of their life. Analysed here first separately then jointly, they show the family circumstances of the childhood and adolescence of people born between 1930 and 1950 as measured against their personal assessments. Various methods were used here, combining data analysis with simple inferential analyses and multilevel analysis.

II – Children and adolescents isolated from family

9We first reconstructed the respondents’ contact circles from birth to age 21, in childhood and adolescence, for each year lived. The contact circle taken into account for this analysis consisted of all immediate family members (parents, other parent figures, siblings, any spouse, any children) with whom the respondent had co-resided or been in frequent contact, using the same definition of “restricted family network” as in our earlier work (Golaz and Lelièvre, 2009 and 2012). The quantity and precision of the information on the respondents’ close kin depended on the relationship between them. Consequently, the restricted family network was reconstructed according to precise rules – described in Golaz and Lelièvre (2012) – for assessing the presence or absence of a relationship between the respondent and each of these people at a given time in his/her past life. In the first place, we included in our calculations people with whom the respondent had lived (including his/her parents and any other parent figures mentioned, his/her spouse, siblings and children). In addition, we also included people who, according to the data, were in regular contact with the respondent.

1 – Times of isolation for respondents in childhood and adolescence

10This restricted family network of Île-de-France residents born between 1930 and 1950 comprised an average of four people during childhood, rose to more than six at ages 30-50, then gradually fell back to fewer than five people as the respondent reached the age of 70 (Golaz and Lelièvre, 2012). On average, over the whole life course, the respondent’s siblings and children were the most numerous members of the restricted family network.

11During childhood and adolescence, parents and siblings dominated, although in the second period spouses and children began to appear in some respondents’ lives (Figure 1).

Figure 1

Figure 1

Figure 1

Average restricted family network of young persons resident in Île-de-France and born between 1930 and 1950, during childhood and adolescence
Source: Biographies et Entourage survey, 2001.

12The respondent’s situation was determined by cross-linking data on the presence (co-residence or living nearby) of the people concerned each year. Reconstructing a contact circle in this way does not depend on direct description; rather, the reconstruction process is based on the cross-linking of precise factual data, allowing maximum comparability between respondents. This reveals periods of relative isolation during childhood or adolescence, when the respondents were in contact with one person in the immediate family at most, but were not necessarily living with them. The criterion applied thus depends on a de facto situation: being isolated for at least one year of childhood/adolescence. The case of Odette is an example of a life course that included a period of isolation (Figure 2).

Figure 2

Figure 2

Figure 2

Odette’s restricted family network during childhood and adolescence
Source: Biographies et Entourage survey, 2001.

13During her childhood, from the ages of 2 to 8, Odette did not live or have regular contact with anyone in her family. At that time, Odette lived in an orphanage, where she had been placed after her father went to prison; she was cut off from her mother and separated from her two brothers so she no longer had any contact with her family members. For Odette, this period was “difficult”, but was followed by some “very good years” when, at age 9, she entered a foster home where she was reunited with her brothers and where an unrelated person played a strong parental role. One of her brothers died young. In the meantime, she gained a vocational qualification in hairdressing and started working. At the end of her adolescence, she got married.

14Over the whole sample, 5.9% of individuals, on average, were affected by a period of relative isolation at each age (period of one year minimum). This proportion decreases to under 3% at ages 5-9. It is reasonable to assume that for a child (in this study, ages 0-12), having a contact circle limited to one person at most – who was not even necessarily co-resident – indicates difficult circumstances. In adolescence (in this study, ages 13-21), the proportion of years of isolation increases again. From age 15, there is a big difference between men (9%) and women (6%), in part because young women form unions earlier, which extends their contact circle, and because young men relatively more frequently leave to take up an apprenticeship, which separates them from their immediate family (Figure 3).

Figure 3

Figure 3

Figure 3

Proportion of Île-de-France residents born between 1930 and 1950 with small family circles during childhood and adolescence
Note: Respondents with small family circles in childhood and adolescence were those who reported being in contact with one person at most from their immediate family at these ages.
Source: Biographies et Entourage survey, 2001.

15Cases like Odette’s – where the close family circle shrank to the point of disappearing for several years of childhood – were not very frequent. These periods during which the family was totally absent represent only 17% of situations of childhood isolation. Most of the time, at least one person from the immediate family remained in contact with the respondent. In more than three quarters of such situations, the close family circle consisted of one parent (60%) or one parent figure (16%). By contrast, in only 40% of situations of adolescent isolation were the individuals in contact with a parent or parent figure; total isolation was more frequent in adolescence (27%) than in childhood (Table 2).

Table 2

Distribution (%) of periods of isolation by composition of close family circle

Table 2
Period starting in Significance of differences between childhood and adolescence childhood adolescence Number of periods of isolation 258 282 Presence of or contact with one parent (father or mother) 160 135 *** another parent figure 116 115 *** a sibling 117 120 *** a spouse – 112 *** No restricted family network 117 127 ** Total 100 100

Distribution (%) of periods of isolation by composition of close family circle

Significance levels: * 5%; ** 1%; *** 0.1%.
Source: Biographies et Entourage survey, 2001.

2 – Contact circle outside the family during periods of isolation

16The Biographies et Entourage survey respondents were also asked to mention anyone who had played an important role – positive or negative – in their life, apart from people in their restricted family network. These were retrospective judgements, dated according to major life periods, including childhood and adolescence, and made with some 40 years’ hindsight. Over the respondents’ lifetimes, it was friends who dominated the social network outside the immediate family. When the presence of such people during childhood was mentioned, they were mainly more distant relatives (62%), who were generally female and older (Lelièvre et al., 2008). This effect diminished over the life course: in adolescence, relatives represented only 39% of the people mentioned outside the restricted family network.

17For respondents who had experienced a period of relative isolation from family during childhood or adolescence (i.e. contact with at most one member of their immediate family for more than one year), there is a lesser propensity to mention people they were close to during these periods. In fact, more than half (53%) of those who had experienced such periods during childhood did not mention any other important person in their contact circle at those times, versus only 48% of people who did not have this experience. In adolescence, the proportions were 55% and 49%, respectively. Thus, isolation from the family seems to go hand in hand with the memory of more general isolation, and the difference between perceptions of childhood and perceptions of adolescence is slight.

18Remaining in the area of perceptions, we will now look at the division of the respondents’ lives into major periods. We will assess the tone of these periods and analyse the comments made by respondents in the Biographies et Entourage survey to justify their opinions.

III – Perception of childhood and adolescent years

19The respondents, aged 50-70 at the time of the survey, and looking back over their life course, were asked to describe in detail their perceptions of their childhood and adolescent years. Drawing on this second type of data, our aim is to examine the childhood and adolescence of these birth cohorts from an original perspective, by looking at the major periods of ego’s life and his/her assessment of it.

20Overall, retrospective perceptions of different times in the lives of the 1930-1950 birth cohorts tended to fluctuate and were structured around factual events – occupational, residential, family and health events – relating to the respondent and his/her close family but also more broadly to the neighbourhood or the society to which the individual belonged. From the many facts of this type, each person makes choices in order to recount and interpret his/her life (Hareven and Masaoka, 1988). Analysis of our data shows that although periods alternated in terms of well-being, a dominant positive note emerges and interpretations were highly diverse. No period of life (childhood, retirement, etc.) nor any event (migration or armed conflict, for example) was systematically perceived as happy or unhappy: “The meanings individuals confer on events and on the unfolding of their lives is neither stereotypical nor predictable” (Laborde et al., 2007).

1 – Periods of childhood and adolescence perceived as “difficult”

21Respondents divided their life courses into between one and twelve periods. Overall, the dominant tone over the lifetime was one of good years (G = 33%). The most frequent “judgement trajectories” were G, GDG, DG and GD, with alternating periods of good years (G) and difficult years (D) (Laborde et al., 2007). Negative judgements represented slightly over a quarter of the periods discussed. However, the average length of periods varied according to the type of judgement. The most difficult periods tended to be shorter than the average.

22Some 15-16% of childhood years were judged to be difficult and only 5-6% very difficult (Table 3). There were more problem years during adolescence than during childhood, with about a quarter of years reported as difficult or very difficult. While judgements relating to childhood were very similar for both genders, differences were perceptible at adolescence. The proportion of difficult years was higher for adolescent girls (26.9%) than for adolescent boys (23.7%), and these differences are significant. The same trend is observed for years reported to be very difficult, which are more frequent for young women. If we compare the difficulties experienced in childhood and adolescence for each gender, women more often perceived their adolescence as difficult or very difficult. For men, these differences were smaller; their childhood seems to be the period during which they had the most “very difficult” times.

Table 3

Proportion of years of life judged as “difficult” or “very difficult”, by gender and life period

Table 3
Number of years of life Proportion of difficult or very difficult years (%) Significance of differences Proportion of very difficult years (%) Significance of differences Between boys and girls Between childhood and adolescence Between boys and girls Between childhood and adolescence Childhood Girls (N=1,487) 19,331 21.6 ns girls *** boys *** 5.3 *** girls *** boys *** Boys (N=1,343) 17,459 21.1 6.0 Adolescence Girls (N=1,487) 10,409 26.9 ** 7.0 *** Boys (N=1,343) 09,401 23.7 5.5 Lifetime 20.2 5.3

Proportion of years of life judged as “difficult” or “very difficult”, by gender and life period

Significance levels: * 5%; ** 1%; *** 1%; ns: not significant.
Source: Biographies et Entourage survey, 2001.

2 – Word association of terms relating to isolation and difficulties experienced

23Going beyond a summary assessment of successive periods in respondents’ life histories, the questionnaire also recorded a description of each period (Questions Sy 1 and Sy 2, Box 1). This enabled us to conduct a textual analysis linking the words used by respondents in describing the periods of their childhood and adolescence to their judgement of these periods. For this, the text of the respondents’ comments was first lemmatized. [2] The lexis associated with discourse on childhood (or on adolescence) was then reduced by grouping words with the same root and eliminating connectives. An aggregated lexical table was then constructed, matching these words to the tone of the judgements and to the sequence of tones that characterized the period. Several qualifiers may follow one another through a period: for example, D then G – a difficult life course that improved – or G – a period of good years. We then used this contingency table to make a multidimensional descriptive analysis of the words obtained, by means of correspondence analysis (CA). [3]

A childhood dominated by family, school and war

24Table 4 (reproduced here only for the period of childhood) shows the words most frequently used to describe the period. Three main themes stand out in descriptions of childhood: family (notably parents), war and school. This period was mostly described as happy, as a carefree period, although some people found it difficult and reported problems. Despite some disparities, the comments on childhood show many similarities, at least in relation to these three main themes.

Table 4

List of words used for the period of childhood, in order of frequency

Table 4
Words used Frequencies Happy + 438 Family + 355 Parent + 353 Good + 230 Father 201 Good 201 Difficult + 196 Problem + 180 War + 176 Mother 164 School 119 Study + 108 Carefree + 173

List of words used for the period of childhood, in order of frequency

Note: The + sign means that variant forms of this root were included when the number of uses was calculated.
Source: Biographies et Entourage survey, 2001.

25In addition, for each respondent’s childhood, the sequence of tones in the judgements was coded in the form of a 6-category variable (D, G, PF, DG, GD and Others). We then constructed the aggregated lexical table by matching each of these types of period to the vocabulary of the discourse on that same period, on which we performed a correspondence analysis. The words in the description of childhood provided by the respondents create a map onto which we can project the types of childhood period in order to study the distribution of vocabulary in given subpopulations (Garnier and Guérin, 2010).

26In Figure 4, childhood trajectories are mapped onto the specific discourse of childhood, and this provides an answer to the question: what is the vocabulary associated with a given life course in childhood? (Garnier and Guérin, 2010). While our preliminary textual analysis of the whole corpus – which by definition related to the whole lifetime – revealed a strong distinction between male and female trajectories (results not shown here), we did not, for childhood, distinguish individuals’ trajectories by gender, since for that period – and only for that one – the type of discourse adopted was not marked by gender differences. There are three distinct focal areas in Figure 4:

  • a difficult childhood (north-east on the map);
  • a good or problem-free childhood (west on the map);
  • a childhood made up of contrasted periods (DG, GD or Others) (south-east on the map).

Figure 4

Figure 4

Figure 4

First factorial map derived from analysing correspondences on the lexical table of terms used to describe childhood, associated with life-course characteristics
Source: Biographies et Entourage survey, 2001.

27The strongest contribution to Axis 1 (51%) corresponds to difficult reported trajectories. Individuals who described their childhood as difficult were more likely to mention themes related to the material conditions of day-to-day life (“money”, “poverty”, “lack”) and to “work” than other respondents; they put more emphasis on the fact that this was “difficult” or “hard”; they used the adjectives “bad” and “unhappy”. In these difficult periods, family relationships were mentioned, as was their absence: “father”, “mother”, “death”.

28Then came trajectories reported as good and problem-free (37%). Those who remembered a problem-free, even good, childhood, talked about their “family” in the broad sense, about “education”, about feeling “carefree”, about “holidays” and “friends”. They tended to use the following adjectives: “caring”, “quiet”, “nice”, “happy”.

29Respondents who reported that their childhood had been more changeable (major contribution to Axis 2 – 46%), had improved (from difficult to good – DG) or, conversely, had deteriorated (GD) covered ambivalent themes. They talked about “school” and “boarding school”, “freedom”, “war” and “grandparents”. Thus, the terms reflect contradictory perceptions: some people felt that going to boarding school was synonymous with freedom, while for others this separation from home was a very bad experience; similarly, “grandparents” might reflect absence of parents or support from grandparents.

Emerging gender differences in adolescence

30For the period of adolescence, the frequency of the words used (table not shown here) again reveals the favoured position of the theme of family, with mention not only of “parents” but also of “brothers” and “sisters”. Siblings seem to become more important during this period. Setting aside “war” – still present here, as was “Algeria” – two threads can be distinguished: “college” and “work”. Some people mention “work”, “job” or “military service”, while others continue to talk about their education (“study”, “boarding school”). For others again, it is already a matter of “marriage”, “couple” and “children”. As in childhood, the adjectives “happy”, “hard”, and “nice” are found. Adolescence is also associated with the themes of “freedom”, “independence”, “discovery” and “holidays”.

31Comments made by respondents about adolescence appear to be strongly gendered. To take this into account, we performed an analysis that included not only the tone of the period, but also the respondent’s gender. This time we constructed a 12-category variable. On the map shown in Figure 5, the tonal variables are qualified: F_D means female adolescence reported as difficult, M_G, male adolescence judged to be good. Distribution of the adjectives in Figure 5 clearly highlights the gendered nature of the respondents’ life courses: all the adjectives on the women’s trajectories are to the left of the vertical axis and all those used by men are to the right (Figure 5). In terms of contribution to the axes, female and male trajectories are balanced on either side of Axis 2. The contribution to Axis 1 is greater for M_G (35%) than for F_D (22%); in contrast, F_G trajectories contribute 33% to Axis 2 and M_D, 19%.

Figure 5

Figure 5

Figure 5

First factorial map derived from the correspondence analysis on the lexical table of terms used to describe adolescence, associated with combined characteristics of periods and respondent’s gender
Source: Biographies et Entourage survey, 2001.

32From adolescence onwards, women in these birth cohorts have a greater tendency to focus on the home, while men focus on work.

33The men who had gone through a difficult period in adolescence covered the themes of “boarding school”, “war” and “work”. Women who described this period as difficult mentioned their “mother” and “illness”. For trajectories that improved (DG), perceptions and vocabularies seem to differ between men and women.

34For those who gave a more positive account of their adolescence (Figure 5, north on the map), regardless of gender, the themes of “meeting” and “discovery” appear. For young women only, the terms “marriage” and “family” were strongly associated when talking about events outside the home environment. Men in the same type of trajectory mentioned “college”, “friends” or feeling “carefree”.

35This textual analysis clearly shows that the contact circle – its absence as much as its presence – played a large part in descriptions of periods of childhood and adolescence. Nevertheless, its role also appears ambivalent. Parents and siblings were mentioned very frequently in both good and difficult periods, indicating that they were cited for their presence and their support as well as for their absence. Girls and boys – or rather, the women and men they have become – have different memories of their adolescence – the period when gender distinctions emerge.

IV – Facts and perceptions: isolation and difficult periods

36In order to clarify the circumstances in which these declarations were made, we will now compare the characteristics of individuals’ close family circles with the respondents’ perceptions of them.

37During childhood and adolescence, years were reported as difficult two or three times more frequently when the close family circle was limited to at most one person (Table 5). Difficult periods coincide with periods when there was little family presence more frequently for girls than for boys, in particular during adolescence.

Table 5

Proportion of years of life during childhood and adolescence judged to be “very difficult”, among years of relative isolation, by gender and period

Table 5
Percentage of “very difficult” years Significance of differences In total number of years In years of relative isolation Between years of isolation and other years Between girls and boys in years of relative isolation Childhood Girls (N=1,487) 5.3 (19,331) 15.8 (657) *** ns Boys (N=1,343) 6.0 (17,459) 12.4 (558) *** Adolescence Girls (N=1,487) 7.0 (10,409) 14.6 (677) *** * Boys (N=1,343) 5.5 (9,401)0 11.0 (865) ***

Proportion of years of life during childhood and adolescence judged to be “very difficult”, among years of relative isolation, by gender and period

Note: Number of years is shown in brackets.
Significance levels: * 5%; ** 1%; *** 0.1%; ns: not significant.
Source: Biographies et Entourage survey, 2001.

1 – Presence of parent figures at difficult times

38To examine this first observation in more detail, we used logistic regression to analyse the effect of the characteristics of the respondent’s restricted family network on judgements of the periods of childhood and adolescence. In order to do this, each period identified by the respondents constitutes a statistical individual. The dependent variable was the fact of considering the period as “difficult” or “very difficult”. Respondent-related control variables (birth cohort/place of birth) and control variables relating to distinctive characteristics of the period (length) and of the contact circle (size and composition of restricted family network) were introduced. In all cases, period length and judgement of the period were correlated, with difficult periods being generally shorter than others. Periods beginning during childhood and completed in adulthood were not taken into account in this work, which leads to slight over-representation of difficult periods (the shortest) by comparison with periods that were judged positively (the longest). Nevertheless, we assume that this has a negligible influence on this first model.

39During childhood and adolescence (Table 6), birth cohort was significant only for boys. Overall, boys born in the 1940s experienced happier periods than boys born in the 1930s. This probably reflects the effect of the war and of the subsequent period of reconstruction. Men born in Île-de-France judged their childhood and adolescence more positively than those born in the provinces or abroad. Moving away from the place of birth showed a significant association with negative assessment of the period, although only for men. This suggests three hypotheses: either the episode of migration or the arrival in Île-de-France was a bad experience, or the migration came at the end of a difficult period, or men tended to retrospectively devalue the part of their childhood spent away from the Île-de-France region where they were living at the time of the survey.

40In contrast, parents’ occupations (results not shown here) had no significant effect on judgements of the period: regardless of their father’s or their mother’s occupational status during their childhood and adolescence, men and women assessed these periods in similar ways. Before age 21, as we have already seen, the immediate family mainly comprised parents, other parent figures and siblings. The presence or absence of siblings does not seem to play any role in judgements of the period (results not shown). People with a large number of siblings did not report their childhood and adolescence as having been more or less happy than those who were the only child in the family. Neither the average size of the immediate family nor the general trend in its size over the period had any significant effect on the respondent’s judgement (results not shown). We therefore constructed two variables describing the family network, one relating to the presence of parents and the other to the presence of parent figures. Women’s judgement of their childhood and adolescence was sensitive only to the presence or absence of parents in the network (periods spent with both parents were significantly happier). Being separated not only from parents but also from all other members of the immediate family also led to an increase in negative judgements. On the other hand, the presence of other parent figures did not play any significant role for girls. Among boys, we find the same influence of the presence of parents, but also a significant relationship with the presence of parent figures. The difficult periods experienced by men during childhood were associated with the presence of parent figures who had helped them to overcome their difficulties. This analysis highlights the importance of the presence of both parents, both in the contact circle and in the household, as well as the significant presence of other parent figures during periods judged to be more difficult than others, in particular for boys.

41Finally, regarding the contact circle outside the family network, 62% of respondents who judged at least one year of their childhood to be “very difficult” did not report any important person in their life during that period. This was the case for 53% of respondents who judged at least one year of childhood to be “difficult” and only 49% of those who did not report any difficult period in childhood. In adolescence, the trend was the same: 57% of respondents who had judged at least one year to be “difficult” or “very difficult” did not report any important person, versus 51% of those who reported no periods of difficulty.

42In order to take the analysis further, some additional characteristics of individual trajectories, such as death of a parent or place of residence, would have to be entered into the earlier logistic model. But because of the personal way in which different individuals divided up their life course trajectories, and because of the variable size of the periods that resulted, this cannot be done without averaging over the periods or making choices that would bias or impoverish the analysis considerably: for the death of a parent, the same category would have to include periods of one year centred on the parent’s death and much longer periods where the death was not necessarily the sole determinant of the overall tone. In the Biographies et Entourage survey, since data were mainly recorded by year, the year was the greatest common divisor of periods (periods which were difficult or not, periods in work, periods of residence, etc.). By analysing situations at the year level, we can evaluate even more fully the relative impact of factors that contribute to creating a bleaker tone for a given period.

2 – Family separation: a factor in judging certain years as “difficult”

43To model the probability of a year of childhood or adolescence being reported as “difficult” or “very difficult”, we first used a simple logistic model (Table 7, Model 3). However, because of the hierarchical structuring of the data – already highlighted for the preliminary analysis by period (Table 6) and appearing even more strongly at the level of years of life (21 years per individual, grouping childhood and adolescence, with characteristics of the year and characteristics of the individual) – a multilevel model becomes necessary (Table 7, Models 4, 5 and 6). The simple model matches characteristics at the “individual” level to the “year” level, so the estimation of their coefficients is incorrect. Yet what interested us here was to distinguish not so much what differentiates one individual from another (we are working on perceptions – and, what is more, retrospective ones – so we can expect large differences between individuals that are difficult to explain), but rather the elements within individual trajectories that influence the tone of one year in comparison to the tone of another year. The multilevel approach, by dissociating calculations at each “individual” and “year” level, enables us to handle this data structuring correctly and thus to answer our question.

Table 6

Relative probabilities (odds ratios) of experiencing a difficult period in childhood and adolescence, by characteristics of ego and of his/her restricted family network

Table 6
Model 1 Model 2 Men Women Characteristics of ego Ego’s year of birth 1930-1935 Ref. Ref. 1936-1940 – 0.27 ns 0.12 ns 1941-1945 – 0.49 ** – 0.06 ns 1946-1950 – 0.42 * – 0.07 ns Place of birth Abroad or French overseas departments and territories Ref. Ref. Île-de-France – 0.50 ** – 0.01 ns Provinces – 0.35 * – 0.00 ns Characteristics of period Length of period Short Ref. Ref. Medium – 0.61 *** – 0.77 *** Long – 0.99 *** – 1.16 *** Characteristics of restricted family network Size of restricted family network over the period Down to 0 for at least one year Ref. Ref. Always greater than 0 – 0.09 ns – 0.67 * Number of parents present At least one parent, but not two for whole period Ref. Ref. No parent for at least one year – 0.20 ns – 0.32 ns 2 parents for whole period – 0.39 ** – 0.38 ** Other parent figures At least one other parent figure Ref. Ref. No other parent figure – 0.50 *** – 0.19 ns

Relative probabilities (odds ratios) of experiencing a difficult period in childhood and adolescence, by characteristics of ego and of his/her restricted family network

Interpretation: Men who mentioned at least one other parent figure described their childhood significantly more favourably than others did, all other things being equal (the odds ratio cannot be interpreted as a relative risk).
Significance levels: * 5%; ** 1%; *** 0.1%; ns: not significant.
Source: Biographies et Entourage survey, 2001.

44In Models 4 to 6, the variance between individuals is very strong and significant (11.2; 11.6; 10.9), as we had expected (Table 7). Taking the hierarchical data structure into account alters the relative weight of some of the explanatory variables in these models. In the logistic model, gender and cohort were significant; in the multilevel models, they were no longer so. Characteristics relating to parents’ occupations also became less significant. The relative balance of the explanatory variables nevertheless differs slightly by gender and, in the light of our preceding observations, different models for each gender yield results that are interesting to compare. The following comments therefore relate to the results of the multilevel models – those which best answer our questions.

Table 7

Effect of characteristics on reporting of difficult or very difficult years in childhood and adolescence

Table 7
Model 3 Logistic Multilevel models Model 4 Model 5 Model 6 Overall Overall Girls Boys Coef. Coef. Coef. Coef. Constant – 1.45 *** – 2.20 *** – 1.46 * – 2.71 *** YEAR level Year of an isolation phase lasting at least two years Ref. Ref. Ref. Ref. Normal year – 0.53 *** – 0.47 ** – 0.46 ** – 0.52 *** Death of one parent in adjacent years [y-1, y, y+1] Ref. Ref. Ref. Ref. No death of close kin – 0.72 *** – 0.66 *** – 0.53 *** – 0.78 *** War (1939-1945) War year Ref. Ref. Ref. Ref. Other year – 0.21 *** – 0.65 *** – 0.59 *** – 0.73 *** Period of life Adolescence (ages 13-20) Ref. Ref. Ref. Ref. Childhood (ages 6-12) – 0.15 *** – 0.52 *** – 0.68 *** – 0.34 *** Early childhood (ages 0-5) – 0.36 *** – 1.10 *** – 1.41 *** – 0.75 *** Place of residence Île-de-France Ref. Ref. Ref. Ref. Provinces – 0.06 * – 0.07 ns 0.06 ns – 0.19 ns Abroad – 0.34 *** – 1.17 *** – 1.06 *** – 1.29 *** INDIVIDUAL level Gender Female Ref. Ref. Male – 0.11 *** – 0.24 ns Cohort 1930-35 Ref. Ref. Ref. Ref. 1936-40 – 0.04 ns – 0.04 ns 0.17 ns – 0.13 ns 1941-45 – 0.17 *** – 0.29 ns 0.05 ns – 0.69 ns 1946-50 – 0.07 * – 0.08 ns 0.07 ns – 0.24 ns Place of birth Abroad Ref. Ref. Ref. Ref. Provinces – 0.35 *** – 1.12 *** – 0.76 ** – 1.43 *** Île-de-France – 0.54 *** – 1.42 *** – 0.81 * – 2.00 *** Father’s occupation Economically inactive Ref. Ref. Ref. Ref. Farmer – 0.14 ** – 0.41 ns – 0.64 ns – 0.27 ns Self-employed – business owner – 0.35 *** – 0.68 * – 0.99 * – 0.35 ns Intermediate occupation – manager – 0.53 *** – 1.07 *** – 0.96 * – 1.24 ** Clerical worker – 0.55 *** – 1.09 *** – 1.11 ** – 1.06 * Manual worker – 0.25 *** – 0.60 * – 0.61 ns – 0.56 ns Mother’s occupation Manual worker Ref. Ref. Ref. Ref. Clerical worker – 0.35 *** – 0.70 * – 0.40 ns – 1.13 * Other employment – 0.42 *** – 0.72 * – 0.45 ns – 1.02 * No paid work – 0.30 *** – 0.56 * – 0.47 ns – 0.69 ns Variance of the constant 11.16 *** 11.56 *** 10.91 ***

Effect of characteristics on reporting of difficult or very difficult years in childhood and adolescence

Note: For each individual, 21 successive years are included in the models. Here, the significant non-zero variances of the constant confirm the validity of the multilevel estimate.
Significance levels: * 5%; ** 1%; *** 0.1%; ns: not significant.
Source: Biographies et Entourage survey, 2001.

45The factors that distinguish individuals are their parents’ occupations and their place of birth. Birth cohort (and gender, Model 4) do not introduce significant differences into the judgements given for childhood and adolescent years. For all cohorts combined, the individuals in our sample who were born abroad, had an inactive father and a mother in a manual occupation were those who gave the most sombre assessments of their childhood and adolescence. The most positive assessments came from individuals born in Île-de-France, with a father in a management, intermediate or clerical occupation and a mother who was working but not a manual worker.

46Within individual histories, the factors that distinguish one year from another are characteristics of the restricted family network (objectively isolated; a year close to the death of a parent), the historical context (Second World War), the period of life in question (adolescence) and finally the individual’s place of residence recorded for each year of his/her life.

47Place of birth and place of residence during the year under consideration have opposite effects: being born abroad is associated with difficult childhood and adolescent years, whereas living abroad is a positive factor. This allows us, for example, to distinguish people born abroad who lived in France during their childhood and adolescence (who combine more factors associated with reporting difficult years) from those living abroad. This result is particularly marked for boys. In addition, for both genders, the adolescent years are significantly more difficult than the childhood years, and early childhood (ages 0-5) is less difficult than the later childhood years (ages 6-12). War and the death of a parent are very negative factors, with a stronger influence on the tone of the life course for boys than for girls. Here we should recall that respondents were aged 15 at most in 1945. The boys lived through the war in similar conditions to the girls, but they were more marked by their experience.

48In all the models, there is a strong link between isolation from family and reported difficulty. Relative isolation is accompanied by a negative perception of that year. However, it is not the sole explanation for a given year having a negative tone, since other factors are involved, such as war, being uprooted, or social background. While this analysis confirms the existence of a clear link between being separated from members of one’s close family and a negative assessment of the period, it also puts this finding in perspective.

Conclusion

49This comparative study of facts and perceptions expands on previous work based on the Biographies et Entourage survey (Lelièvre et al., 2006; Laborde et al., 2007; Golaz and Lelièvre, 2009).

50In this article, we have looked more specifically at the influence of family situation on the respondents’ retrospective assessments of periods in their childhood and their adolescence. We first described times when the family network was limited to at most one person and then we went on to study, on the basis of quantitative data and of terminology widely used by the respondents, the characteristics of periods defined by the survey as “difficult” or “very difficult”. Finally, we compared the reality of having a small contact circle with the perceived feeling that things were difficult.

51Periods when the restricted family network (close family with whom one is in contact) comprised just one person at most were more frequent in adolescence than in childhood. During adolescence, young men were more isolated from their families than young women. Moreover, 17% of these episodes in childhood and 27% in adolescence were periods where there was no contact at all with close family. During childhood, when there was contact with one person, in the great majority of cases this was a parent or a person playing a parental role. In adolescence, the presence of parents diminished and was replaced by that of siblings or possibly spouses, in particular for young women. It should also be noted that isolated children and adolescents reported more frequently than others no other important person in their life to replace the immediate family they lacked. Their relative isolation from family was compounded by social isolation.

52In terms of perceptions, childhood and adolescence were assessed overall as more difficult than the rest of the life course for these 1930-1950 birth cohorts. One fifth of childhood years were reported by the respondents as difficult, as were about a quarter of the years of adolescence. Overall, negative perceptions of adolescence were more frequent than negative perceptions of childhood, adolescence being a time when episodes of isolation are most frequent. However, young women recounted more sombre histories than did young men. Family, school and war (a prominent historical characteristic of these birth cohorts) dominate the description of periods of childhood. Difficulty in this period is associated with privation, with parents, with death and with poverty, while happier periods were described using terms relating to family in the broad sense, school, friends and feeling carefree. These judgements, relatively undifferentiated by gender in childhood, become more distinct in adolescence (here, ages 13-21). For men, war, boarding school and work were linked to a negative tone for the period; for young women, the negative terms were illness and close family. Perception trajectories that became more positive were associated with college and with friends for men, with marriage and with family for women. This description clearly demonstrates not only the ambivalent role of close family in perceptions of the period but also the marked differences between men and women, which appear at adolescence.

53Matching facts (a particular family situation) to perceptions (assessment of the period) enables us to establish the link between relative isolation and perceived difficulty. Years of relative isolation were reported as difficult periods two or three times more often than other years. The most difficult years were also the years when, outside the immediate family, the contact circle around the individual became smaller. The most difficult years were more often associated with isolation than other years.

54For both men and women, a small family circle and a negative perception of the period or year are correlated. The contact circle has a significant influence on the tone of the period, as the presence of parents or parent figures showed. Having a small contact circle is, overall, associated with a negative perception of the year, but that is not the sole factor. War or the death of a parent explain the differences in the tone of the recorded trajectories over childhood and adolescence.

55The potential of these matched observations, combining facts and perceptions is clearly illustrated here: they enable us to understand situations more fully. Moreover, although the close family circle is important – and all the more so during childhood and adolescence – these results show the extent to which it represents both an ambivalent resource and an unequally distributed one.

Notes

  • [*]
    Institut national d’études démographiques, Paris.
    Correspondence: Éva Lelièvre, Institut national d’études démographiques (INED), 133 boulevard Davout, 75980 Paris cedex 20, tel.: 00 33 (1) 56 06 21 31, e-mail: eva@ined.fr
  • [1]
    These are other people identified by respondents as having played a parental role (mentioned by 21% of them). These parent figures were mostly female and relatives of the respondent, although 16% of them had no kinship tie, and were either linked to the respondent’s local environment (childminder, teacher, etc.) or established a relationship with him/her during childhood (Lelièvre et al., 2008).
  • [2]
    Lemmatizing means lexical analysis of the contents of a text by grouping of words in the same family. It groups together the various forms that a word can take: noun, plural, conjugated verb or verb in the infinitive, etc.
  • [3]
    This was based on work initiated by Caroline Laborde with the help of Bénédicte Garnier using SPAD-T.
English

Abstract

The Biographies et Entourage (Event histories and contact circle) survey carried out in 2000-2001 on residents of the Paris region born between 1930 and 1950, provides a means to reconstruct the composition of respondents’ contact circles over their lifetimes. It also records their subjective assessments of the different parts of their life course. In this article, we analyse how these subjective data – reconstructed ex post – and the factual situations that characterize periods of the respondents’ childhood and adolescence are interlinked. Periods characterized by the size and composition of the close family circle were studied using complementary approaches ranging from textual analysis to inferential analyses (logistic and multilevel regression). The results show in detail how the close family circle shapes the respondents’ subjective judgements. Textual analysis shows that the contact circle features strongly in descriptions of periods of childhood and adolescence. Even though other factors are involved, periods of relative isolation are perceived more negatively than other periods. Despite gendered differences, which come to light in adolescence, the contact circle has a significant influence on the tone of the period for both boys and girls, and this is revealed particularly through the presence or absence of parents or parent figures.

Keywords

  • France
  • childhood
  • adolescence
  • contact circle
  • isolation
  • perceptions
Français

L’entourage familial pendant l’enfance et l’adolescence, entre faits et perceptions. Une analyse rétrospective des parcours de vie des Franciliens des générations 1930-1950

L’entourage familial pendant l’enfance et l’adolescence, entre faits et perceptions. Une analyse rétrospective des parcours de vie des Franciliens des générations 1930-1950

L’enquête Biographies et entourage, réalisée en 2000-2001 auprès de Franciliens nés entre 1930 et 1950, permet de reconstituer la composition de l’entourage des personnes au cours de leur vie. Elle a également recueilli une évaluation subjective des différentes parties des trajectoires individuelles. Dans cet article, nous analysons comment ces données subjectives reconstruites a posteriori et les situations factuelles qui caractérisent les périodes de l’enfance et de l’adolescence des enquêtés se conjuguent et se complètent. Les périodes caractérisées par la taille et la composition de l’en tourage familial sont étudiées à l’aide d’approches complémentaires, de l’analyse textuelle, jusqu’aux analyses inférentielles (régressions logistiques et multiniveau). Les résultats détaillent la façon dont l’entourage familial marque l’appréciation subjective que formulent les personnes. De l’analyse textuelle il ressort que l’entourage intervient largement dans la qualification des périodes de l’enfance et de l’adolescence. Même si d’autres facteurs interviennent, les périodes d’isolement relatif font l’objet d’une perception plus négative que les autres. Malgré des différences genrées qui voient le jour à l’adolescence, l’entourage a une influence significative sur l’ambiance de la période pour les garçons comme pour les filles, qui transparaît particulièrement à travers la présence ou absence des parents ou de figures parentales.

Español

El entorno familiar durante la infancia y la adolescencia, entre hechos y percepciones. Un análisis retrospectivo de las trayectorias de vida de los habitantes de la región parisina nacidos entre 1930 y 1950

El entorno familiar durante la infancia y la adolescencia, entre hechos y percepciones. Un análisis retrospectivo de las trayectorias de vida de los habitantes de la región parisina nacidos entre 1930 y 1950

La encuesta Biographies et entourage, realizada en 200-2001 entre los habitantes de la región parisina nacidos entre 1930 y 1950, permite reconstituir la composición del entorno familiar de los individuos en el curso de sus vidas. La encuesta ha recogido igualmente una valoración subjetiva de las diferentes etapas de las trayectorias individuales. En este artículo analizamos cómo estos datos subjetivos reconstruidos a posteriori, se conjugan y se completan con las situaciones factuales que caracterizan la infancia y la adolescencia de los encuestados. Los periodos caracterizados por el tamaño y la composición del entorno familiar son estudiados bajo enfoques complementarios, desde el análisis textual hasta los análisis inferenciales (regresiones logísticas y análisis multinivel). Los resultados detallan el modo cómo el entorno familiar influye sobre la apreciación subjetiva que formulan las personas. El análisis textual indica que el entorno interviene ampliamente en la evaluación de los periodos de la infancia y de la adolescencia. Aunque intervienen otros factores, los periodos de aislamiento relativo son percibidos más negativamente que los otros. A pesar de las diferencias relativas al género que aparecen en la adolescencia, el entorno familiar, en particular la presencia o ausencia de los padres o de figuras parentales, tiene una influencia significativa sobre la percepción de ese periodo tanto en los muchachos como en las muchachas.
Translated by Karen George.

References

  • Attias-Donfut C., 1991, Générations et âges de la vie, Paris, PUF, Que sais-je ?, 125 p.
  • OnlineAttias-Donfut C., Sirven N. (eds.), 2009, “Famille et vieillissement en Europe : Enquête SHARE”, Retraite et société, special issue 58, pp. 4-107.
  • OnlineBessin M., Bidart C., Grossetti M. (eds.), 2010, Bifurcations, Paris, La Découverte, Collection Recherches, 397 p.
  • OnlineBonvalet C., Lelièvre E., 1995, “Du concept de ménage à celui d’entourage : une redéfinition de l’espace familial”, Sociologie et sociétés, 27(2), special issue: Une nouvelle morphologie sociale, pp. 177-190.
  • OnlineBonvalet C., Lelièvre E. (eds.), 2012, De la famille à l’entourage. L’enquête Biographies et entourage, Paris, INED, Grandes enquêtes, 472 p.
  • OnlineBonvalet C., Ogg J. (eds.), 2006, Measuring Family Support in Europe, Southern Universities Press, London, 223 p.
  • OnlineFrechon I., Villeneuve-Gokalp C., 2009, “Enfants attendus et enfants accueillis dans le cadre de l’adoption”, Politique sociale et familiale, 95, pp. 33-42.
  • OnlineFrechon I., Guyavarch E., Halifax J., 2009, “État des lieux de l’enfance en danger, sources et données disponibles en France”, Santé, société et solidarité, 8(1), pp. 39-47.
  • OnlineGalambos N., Turner P., Tilton-Weaver L., 2005, “Chronological and subjective age in emerging adulthood: The crossover effect”, Journal of Adolescent Research, 20(5), pp. 538-556.
  • Garnier B., Guérin F., 2010, Appliquer les méthodes de la statistique textuelle, CEPED, Les Clefs pour, 89 p.
  • Golaz V., Lelièvre E., 2009, “Mémoire, appréciation de la vie et entourage familial”, in Marcoux R. (ed.) with Dion J., Mémoire et démographie, regards croisés au Sud et au Nord, Québec, Presses de l’Université Laval, Cahiers du CIEQ, pp. 155-167.
  • Golaz V., Lelièvre E., 2012, “Continuités et ruptures de l’entourage au fil de la vie”, in Schoumaker B., Tabutin D. (eds.), Les systèmes d’information en démographie et en sciences sociales. Nouvelles questions, nouveaux outils ?, Proceedings of the Chaire Quételet 2006, Centre de recherche en démographie et société, Université catholique de Louvain, pp. 213-234.
  • OnlineHareven T., 1986, “Historical changes in the social construction of the life course”, Human Development, 29(3), pp. 171-180.
  • OnlineHareven T., Masaoka K., 1988, “Turning points and transitions: Perceptions of the life course”, Journal of Family History, 13(1), pp. 271-289.
  • Henry L., van de Valle E., 1982, Multilingual Demographic Dictionary, English Section, Liège, Ordina, 161 p.
  • OnlineLaborde C., Lelièvre E., Vivier G., 2007, “Trajectories and landmark events: how people narrate their lives. Analysis of life course facts and perceptions”, Population, English Edition, 62(3), pp. 487-506.
  • Leclerc-Olive M., 1997, Le dire de l’événement (biographique), Presses universitaires du Septentrion, 264 p.
  • OnlineLelièvre E., Vivier G., 2001, “Évaluation d’une collecte à la croisée du quantitatif et du qualitatif, l’enquête Biographies et entourage”, Population, 56(6), pp. 1043-1074.
  • OnlineLelièvre E., Vivier G., Tichit C., 2008, “Between socially-instituted parental relationships and elective ones: a retrospective analysis of parent figures in France from 1930 to 1965”, Population, English Edition, 63(2), pp. 209-238.
  • OnlineLelièvre E., Roubaud F., Tichit C., Vivier G., 2006, “Factual data and perceptions: fuzziness in observation and analysis”, in Lelièvre E., Antoine P. (eds), Fuzzy States and Complex Trajectories, Observation, Modelization and Interpretation of Life Histories, Paris, INED, Méthodes et savoirs, 6, pp. 25-53.
  • OnlineLocke C., Lloyd-Sherlock P., 2011, “Qualitative life course methodologies: Critical reflections from development studies”, Development and Change, 42(5), pp. 1131-1152.
  • Meslé F., Toulemon L., Véron J. (eds.), 2011, Dictionnaire de démographie et des sciences de la population, Paris, Armand Colin, 528 p.
  • OnlinePailhé A., Solaz A., 2009, Entre famille et travail, des arrangements de couples aux pratiques des employeurs, Paris, La Découverte, 512 p.
  • Solsona M., Simó C., 2009, “When a love story ends, which biographies become relevant?”, paper presented at the IUSSP Conference, Marrakech, 27 Sept-2 Oct..
  • Tichit C., Vivier G., 2005, “La pluriparentalité, une ressource familiale méconnue”, Idées, 140, pp. 19-25.
  • OnlineWidmer E., 1999, “Family contexts as cognitive networks: A structural approach of family relationships”, Personal Relationships, 6(4), pp. 487-503.
  • Widmer E., Jallinoja R. (eds.), 2008, Beyond the Nuclear Family: Families in a Configurational Perspective, Peter Lang, Population, Family and Society, vol. 9, 401 p.
  • Widmer E., Chevalier M., Dumas P., 2005, “Le “family network method“ (FNM) : un outil d’investigation des configurations familiales à disposition des thérapeutes”, Thérapie familiale, 26(4), pp. 423-441.
Valérie Golaz [*]
  • [*]
    Institut national d’études démographiques, Paris.
    Correspondence: Éva Lelièvre, Institut national d’études démographiques (INED), 133 boulevard Davout, 75980 Paris cedex 20, tel.: 00 33 (1) 56 06 21 31, e-mail: eva@ined.fr
Éva Lelièvre [*]
  • [*]
    Institut national d’études démographiques, Paris.
    Correspondence: Éva Lelièvre, Institut national d’études démographiques (INED), 133 boulevard Davout, 75980 Paris cedex 20, tel.: 00 33 (1) 56 06 21 31, e-mail: eva@ined.fr
Translated by
Karen George
Uploaded on Cairn-int.info on 03/03/2014
Cite
Distribution électronique Cairn.info pour I.N.E.D © I.N.E.D. Tous droits réservés pour tous pays. Il est interdit, sauf accord préalable et écrit de l’éditeur, de reproduire (notamment par photocopie) partiellement ou totalement le présent article, de le stocker dans une banque de données ou de le communiquer au public sous quelque forme et de quelque manière que ce soit.
keyboard_arrow_up
Chargement
Loading... Please wait